<< Hide Menu
John Mohl
Haseung Jun
Dalia Savy
John Mohl
Haseung Jun
Dalia Savy
Operant conditioning refers to when a behavior leads to an environmental response, which affects the likelihood of the behavior happening again.
B.F. Skinner took this principle further and described different types of consequences that can occur and how they could affect the presentation of the behavior. He created what is called the "Skinner box", where animals in the box only receive food if they press a lever or peck a disk. The food was used as a reinforcer, using the food as reinforcement.
🎥 Watch: AP Psychology—Operant Conditioning with Pigeons
Reinforcing behavior means there is a greater likelihood that the behavior will occur again. Contrarily, punishing a behavior will create a lessened probability that the behavior will happen again. In the box,
Thus, when a behavior is positively reinforced, it means something is presented (usually something pleasant) to increase the likelihood of the behavior happening again. When something is negatively reinforced, something is taken away (usually unpleasant) to encourage that behavior to happen again.
When something is positively punished, something is presented (usually unpleasant), making the behavior happen less often. In contrast, something negatively punished has something taken away (usually something pleasant) to make that behavior happen less often.
Operant Conditioning Term | Description | Outcome | Example |
Positive Reinforcement | Add or increase a pleasant stimulus | Behavior is strengthened | You get a cookie for an “A.” 🍪 |
Negative Reinforcement | Reduce or remove an unpleasant stimulus | Behavior is strengthened | Taking painkillers (removes pain), the behavior of taking painkillers is strengthened. |
Positive Punishment | Add an unpleasant stimulus | Behavior is weakened | Give more homework for misbehavior ✍️ |
Negative Punishment | Reduce or remove pleasant stimulus | Behavior is weakened | No phone 📱 after breaking curfew |
Skinner used negative and positive reinforcements and punishments to train the rats inside the Skinner box. But sometimes, we can't wait forever for the rat to learn. So in order to speed up the process, we can use shaping. Shaping reinforces the steps to reach the end goal. For example, the rat could be reinforced for touching the lever with any part of its body. Because we're rewarding any behavior close to the behavior we want, we have a greater chance of getting the rat to stumble upon the behavior we want.
There's also something called chaining. When the animals are trained enough, they can perform multiple tasks in order to get the reward. A good example of this would be going through an obstacle course to get the final reward.
There are basic conditioning phenomena also describe the process of operant conditioning.
Acquisition | Rat learns to press the food lever for food |
Extinction | The rat unlearns that connection between the lever and food and the rat stops pressing the lever |
Spontaneous Recovery | After a period of rest without the learned behavior, the rat presses the lever |
Generalization | The rat presses anything that looks like a lever, thinking it will give it food |
Discrimination | The rat learns to only press a certain lever |
🎥 Watch: AP Psychology—Positive and Negative Punishments
Despite stringent behaviorists’ claims, there are limitations to classical conditioning. When presented with a puzzle 🧩, some organisms can discover the solution to the problem without proper reinforcements to guide them to the solution. This phenomenon is known as insight learning. Insight learning is sometimes referred to as the “a-ha moment” when one suddenly realizes the solution to a problem💡.
Edward Tolman found that rats did not show any noticeable improvement in getting through a maze in the absence of reinforcement. However, when reinforcement was provided, he found a marked decrease in the time needed to finish the maze, suggesting that the rats knew the solution to the maze but did not express it behaviorally, meaning that they had a cognitive map of the maze. Tolman called this latent learning⏳.
Other research has shown that cognitive interpretations of conditioning also play a role. If a person believes that a particular stimulus, as opposed to the intended stimulus, causes the conditioning, then the stimulus designed to produce the conditioning will not occur.
The probability of successful operative conditioning depends upon how the reinforcements are presented.
When something is produced on a fixed schedule, reinforcement occurs in a predictable (but not continuous) pattern. One knows when the subsequent reinforcement will be given, assuming behaviors are performed. When reinforcement is presented on a variable schedule, it means that reinforcement is not predictable, and it is not apparent when the next reinforcement will exactly occur.
When reinforcement is given on an interval schedule, it means a certain amount of time must pass by, assuming the behavior is performed before reinforcement is given. When reinforcement is given on a ratio schedule, a certain number of behaviors must be performed before the reinforcement is provided.
Altogether, this makes four different types of schedules of reinforcement.
Reinforcement Schedule | Explanation | Real World Example |
Fixed Ratio | Rewarded after a specific number of responses #️⃣ | You get paid $100 bucks after writing two columns. |
Variable Ratio | Rewarded after an average but unpredictable number of responses | Put money in a slot machine. It pays out after several plays, but the player is uncertain of the number because it varies. |
Fixed Interval | Rewarded after a set amount of time has elapsed 📅 | People who earn a monthly salary |
Variable Interval | Rewarded after an average but unpredictable amount of time has elapsed | Person checks email messages and is rewarded with a message at varying times. |
🎥Watch: AP Psychology—Operant Conditioning
© 2024 Fiveable Inc. All rights reserved.